Issue Date: June 18, 2012
Molecular Gastronomy Cooks Up Strange Plate-Fellows
News Channels: Biological SCENE
Keywords: molecular gastronomy, flavor pairing, gas chromatography-olfactometry, flavor chemistry, food chemistry
Interact with this Foodpairing tree at http://cenm.ag/foodpair.
Some foods taste good together, and others don’t. It’s a simple observation rooted in the complex biochemistry of human senses. During the past decade, an idea about why this is the case has been making the rounds among chefs, flavor chemists, and food scientists.
The idea, which has several names but is often called flavor-pairing theory, is that foods that share important flavor compounds are likely to make a good pair. It has spurred a culinary movement and a company dedicated to helping the pros discover innovative pairings through chemistry. However, the concept is still in its infancy, and it’s not without vociferous criticism. Some critics tell C&EN that hard data supporting the idea are lacking and that taste is too complicated to boil down to a molecular rule of thumb. Other critics are more direct, as network analysis expert Maurits de Klepper of Amsterdam University College is in decrying flavor pairing as “a European fad” (Gastronomica, DOI: 10.1525/gfc.2012.11.4.55).
The flavor-pairing concept traces back to a partnership between François Benzi, senior flavor chemist at the world’s largest privately owned flavor house, Firmenich, and Heston Blumenthal, owner of three-Michelin-starred U.K. restaurant The Fat Duck. The two share a knack for unconventional food combinations, such as pork liver and jasmine flower or white chocolate and caviar. Thanks to fragrance chemistry expertise and analytical instrumentation, they realized their pairings have key flavor compounds in common—indole in the pork-jasmine case, and amines in the white chocolate-caviar combo.
Inspired, Blumenthal sought additional food combinations, guided by information on the volatile organic compounds in hundreds of different foods, since the sense of smell, which detects those volatiles, plays a big part in the sense of taste. Soon, lists of Blumenthal’s outlandish pairings made their way around culinary and flavor science circles, as well as the Web.
That’s where Norweigan organometallic chemist Martin Lersch learned about Benzi and Blumenthal’s exploits. Lersch, who runs the molecular gastronomy blog Khymos, started a flavor-pairing blog event, called “They Go Really Well Together,” in 2007. At the start of each event, he notified food bloggers of one unusual food duo that share volatile compounds and challenged them to craft appetizing dishes from that pairing.
For instance, one combo Lersch put forward was strawberry and coriander, after he deduced from the literature that the two share at least one key odorant, (Z)-3-hexenal (J. Agric. Food Chem., DOI: 10.1021/jf960366o; “Handbook of Flavor Characterization: Sensory Analysis, Chemistry, and Physiology,” CRC Press, 2004). Bloggers who tried it confirmed the combination was tasty.
Lersch notes that odorant overlap alone wasn’t a guarantee of success—during some events, bloggers had to tweak recipes quite a bit to balance flavors. But in any case, kitchen experiments can’t demonstrate that shared volatile compounds are a reliable guide to whether foods will pair well.
That seems to be the goal of Sense for Taste, a privately held firm that provides consulting services for chefs, bartenders, and food companies on developing innovative pairings. Sense for Taste cofounder Bernard Lahousse, a bioengineer by training, says his company is out to develop a science inspired by Benzi’s flavor chemist instincts. The firm’s researchers analyze foods and use a proprietary algorithm to calculate the compatibilities of possible pairings. They then translate those data, kept in an in-house database, into easy-to-grasp visualizations called Foodpairing trees, which provide an overview of possible combinations with one ingredient.
The Belgium-based company has played matchmaker for several multinational brands. Its recipe for almond sponge cake with poached banana, chocolate, and Heinz tomato ketchup ice cream is generating buzz on Facebook, Lahousse says. “People want to be surprised.”
Lahousse confirmed that the company uses high-performance liquid chromatography and gas chromatography/mass spectrometry but declined to elaborate on other methods. The algorithms, he says, take into account more than the number of volatile compounds two foods share. Human senses perceive different compounds at different thresholds, so things must be weighted appropriately, he says.
Not all scientists are sold on the flavor-pairing concept. For instance, last December, a team suggested that the idea can’t explain every cuisine (Sci. Rep., DOI: 10.1038/srep00196). Led by theoretical physicist and amateur molecular gastronomist Sebastian E. Ahnert of Cambridge University, the scientists developed a network linking flavor compounds to ingredients in more than 50,000 online recipes grouped by cuisine, including North American, Latin American, or East Asian cuisines. Their network unearthed an intriguing contrast. North American recipes use far more compound-sharing pairs than would be expected by chance. For instance, mozzarella, tomato, and parmesan share 4-methylpentanoic acid. However, East Asian recipes rarely share key flavor compounds.
Lahousse says he doesn’t agree with the physicists’ results. The databases that are the foundation of the work, he explains, equally weight the entire complement of odorant compounds from GC/MS analysis of a food, rather than favoring those key molecules that drive the taste experience. Those compounds are traditionally determined by techniques such as GC-olfactometry, which involves a live person sniffing separate compounds and rating contributions to smell or taste sensations. “If they had our data they would’ve come to a different conclusion,” Lahousse says.
Ahnert says he and his colleagues chose their approach so they’d be able to attach statistical significance to the results. He agrees with Lahousse that the odorant data the team used are incomplete. Now that their network analysis method is hammered out, they are collecting data that will distinguish more important odor compounds from less important ones, something he thinks will be critical in confirming and better understanding what they observed in Eastern cuisines.
According to Wender L. P. Bredie, a professor of sensory science at the University of Copenhagen, the flavor-pairing concept needs refining. He thinks pairings that result from such experiments are more novel than they are pleasant, and he has preliminary data to back it up.
Bredie’s team used a commercial database to estimate the overlap of volatiles in 53 pairs of foods. His team then placed the food pairs in glass vials, separated by a small piece of aluminum foil. A panel of 40 volunteers sniffed the combinations and rated their intensity, pleasantness, and novelty. If the flavor-pairing principle were at work, Bredie says, one would expect to see a correlation with the pleasantness rating and the overlap of volatiles in the two foods. His team saw no such correlation. However, they did notice that certain aroma combinations were deemed more novel than either scent alone. Bredie plans to submit more detailed results to a peer-reviewed publication soon.
“When top chefs combine foods, they and their customers might say, ‘Wow, this is really new!’ But it’s not really pleasant,” Bredie says, because the average consumer’s preferences are tied to culture and previous experiences. Flavor pairing “has engaged people to create new things,” he adds. “In that way it has made positive contributions.”
Interact with this Foodpairing tree at http://cenm.ag/foodpair.
“The biggest problem in this field is really a lack of data,” Lersch says. Most scientists in the field have to rely on commercial databases, as Bredie did, and the data within them aren’t always complete or standardized.
The big barrier preventing academics from pursuing that kind of data themselves is cost, says Peter Barham, a polymer physicist at the University of Bristol, in England, who’s also studied molecular gastronomy. Statistically valid, blinded tests with professionally trained tasters don’t come cheaply, he says. He adds that he was fortunate to be privy to proprietary information when he dabbled in flavor pairing, though he couldn’t publish some of his results.
It’s going to take a lot of data to convince Harry J. Klee that flavor pairing makes scientific sense. “It’s ridiculous,” says Klee, a professor of horticultural science at the University of Florida. “And I think our own work emphasizes how ridiculous it is.” Klee’s lab has been studying tomato flavor for years, and it just published a metabolomics study fingering geranial, a volatile compound that’s present in low amounts in tomatoes, as a big contributor to satisfying tomato flavor (Curr. Biol., DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.016). More abundant volatile compounds had little to do with tomato preferences.
Klee thinks geranial enhances tomato sweetness without adding sugar, raising the possibility that playing it up could enhance sweetness in other foods. That finding, Klee says, illustrates how little scientists know about how the brain integrates taste and aroma signals from multiple receptors to conjure a flavor experience. “That whole flavor-pairing crap is just a gimmick by a chef who is practicing biology without a license.”
“We don’t want to practice biology,” says Peter Smith, chef-proprietor of PS 7’s restaurant in Washington, D.C. “We just want to make really cool flavors and combinations.” If the vehicle happens to be science-based, that’s fantastic. But Smith says tools like Lahousse’s Foodpairing trees are useful from a creative standpoint even if the science isn’t proven. “I’ve been obsessed with them,” Smith says. And he’s got all his cooks studying them for ideas. “In the day-to-day grind of this business you get tunnel vision, and these things pull in ingredients we don’t always think about.”
Lahousse says that the combinations his company provides are just a starting point and that finding the right balance is up to the chef. He also says the flavor-pairing concept isn’t meant to apply to all of cooking—some ingredients in a recipe have little, if anything, to do with flavor. “If you add lemon, you may want to add it because of its flavor, or because of its acidity, or both,” he says.
For Terry E. Acree, the Cornell University scientist who developed the Flavornet database, flavor pairing “is a totally real thing.” Saying it has no basis in science, he says, “is not the same thing as saying the rules are ephemeral.” Flavor pairing, Acree adds, is just one part of the lesson chefs like Blumenthal are teaching the food world. To change the way people eat, cooks need to scientifically understand the materials they’re manipulating.
Firmenich’s Benzi agrees: “I love cooking, and I’ve realized I’ve always unconsciously had this molecular approach to it. If a chef could take the time to experience some of the esters and some of the lactones I work with, by smelling them or tasting them, I’m sure they’d approach the kitchen in a different way.”
- Chemical & Engineering News
- ISSN 0009-2347
- Copyright © American Chemical Society
I have a couple of thoughts with respect to the whole idea/field of flavor pairing.
1) Eating involves flavor perception. The flavor pairing fad presumes that if two foods share a major aroma molecule, how we sense that molecule is enhanced or altered. Following their logic, the presence of molecule A in both food 1 and food 2 changes the way we perceive molecule A in a mixture of the 2. This doesn't make much sense to my chemical and biological intuition. What I would venture is happening is that there are different molecules that are complementary to molecule A in food 1 and food 2. I would imagine that it is the mixtures of these complementary molecules and their interaction with molecule A that has an effect on what and how we perceive molecule A. It seems that this is what Klee is showing in his studies. Gerenial has a plays a really important, and not fully understood role, in the way we perceive "sweetness" in tomatoes. So, it seems to me that finding foods with common molecules might be a good first pass at discovering new complementary interactions, the mere presence of a single molecule in two foods will not necessarily make for a novel dining experience.
2) Scientifically, this idea employs a lot of concepts that we still don't understand. How do structurally similar, non-polar molecules interact with each other in an aqueous matrix. How do cocktails of molecules influence the receptor for an individual molecule found within that cocktail? (Seems that this question has big impacts in pharma world as well.) How does a stable aggregate of two different molecules (molecule A and B) affect how molecule a is perceived? These are really interesting questions with no straightforward experiments. But, taste perception (and understanding networked GCPRs) might be a good foray into these questions with broad scientific interest.
3) Again, taste is about molecular perception. If multiple molecules enhance how molecule A is perceived, the enhancement may be beneficial or detrimental to the overall experience. I think that this is the other scientifically relevant component missing in the discussion of flavor pairing. In other words, mixer beware.
That leads us back to what cooking and haute cuisine is, in general. Haute cuisine (in its current incarnation) is about discovering new ways to stimulate the senses with dining. There is nothing wrong with using the flavor pairing model as a starting point for looking for new flavor experiences. But, there is no scientific reason why the tenets of flavor pairing must lead to something enjoyable. The science is much more interesting and intricate than that!
Thanks for the detailed comments. With respect to (1), one of the things Martin Lersch has blogged about is that the term seems to mean different things to different people. See here: blog.khymos.org/2011/03/08/flavor-pairing-revisited/
Even with all those distinctions, though, I think your question remains unanswered. It seems like it's why so many network analysis folks have become interested in tackling this concept.
One of the things I found most fascinating in interviewing for this story was how many of the chefs/bartenders I interviewed for this story thought that "using the flavor pairing model as a starting point for looking for new flavor experiences" as you put it, was enough. Another was the age-old concept of having a theory that's elegant in its simplicity. Unfortunately, biology is messy, and taste biology particularly so.
I'd be interested to learn more about GC/olfactometry. Is the method something attempted to be standardized amongst molecular gastronomy labs? I would assume that a person's ability to perceive a sample's smell can be affected by seasonal allergies, state of health, what they've eaten recently, or by their own clothes and cosmetics. The community in which a person lives will also sensitive them to specific scents, so that employing a panel of local testers may reduce variability, but may not eliminate bias.
The statement Peter Smith makes is something I wholeheartedly agree with. Even if flavor pairing flops from a formalist standpoint, there is certainly value in expanding the possibilities for pairings through outside input. It reminds me of my incipient days of vegetarianism, when, in order to find foods that satisfied me, I was forced to expand my palette to non-customary (to me) items like quinoa and seitan.
Flavor pairing can be analysed in so many different ways - similar chemical structures could infer enjoyability just as much as dissonant ones could, concentration effects are likely non-trivial, the food host 'matrix' or texture could take a role in the accessibility of molecules-of-interest, and as was stated earlier, solubility may be key as well. I am excited to see the future of 'informed' food pairing.
Thanks again for this read.
-Evan
"Unfortunately, despite advances in electronic noses, our biased biological noses are still the best we've got"
And, thank goodness for that. Our noses are some of the finest pieces of analytical equipment around! And, I for one, thank my lucky stars that this is the case!